APDT

Asset 7@4x

Training Methodologies

Training Methodologies

Training Methodologies

Section 1

Least Instrusive, Minimally Aversive (LIMA)

“LIMA” is an acronym for the phrase “least intrusive, minimally aversive”. LIMA describes a trainer or behavior consultant who uses the least intrusive, minimally aversive strategy out of a set of humane and effective tactics likely to succeed in achieving a training or behavior change objective with minimal risk of producing aversive side effects. LIMA adherence also requires consultants to be adequately educated and skilled in order to ensure that the least intrusive and aversive procedure is used. 1 LIMA does not justify the use of punishment in lieu of other effective interventions and strategies. In the vast majority of cases, desired behavior change can be affected by focusing on the animal’s environment, physical well-being, and operant and classical interventions such as differential reinforcement of an alternative behavior, desensitization, and counter-conditioning.

LIMA requires trainers/consultants to work to increase the use of positive reinforcement and eliminate the use of punishment when working with animal and human clients. In order to ensure best practices, consultants should pursue and maintain competence in animal behavior consulting and training through continuing education, and hands-on experience. Trainers/consultants should not advise on problems outside the recognized boundaries of their competencies and experience. 2

Positive reinforcement should be the first line of teaching, training, and behavior change program considered, and should be applied consistently. Positive reinforcement is associated with the lowest incidence of aggression, attention seeking, avoidance, and fear in learners. 3 Only the learner determines what may be reinforcing. It is crucial that the trainer/consultant understands and has the ability to appropriately apply this principle. This fact may mean that the trainer/consultant assesses any handling, petting, food, tool, and environment each time the learner experiences them. Personal bias must not determine the learner’s experience. The measure of each stimulus is whether the learner’s target behavior is strengthening or weakening, not the trainer/consultant’s intent or preference.

The trainer/consultant is responsible for ensuring learner success through a consistent, systematic approach that identifies a specific target behavior, the purpose of that behavior, and the consequences that maintain the behavior. A variety of learning and behavior change strategies may come into play during a case. Ethical use of this variety always depends on the trainer/consultant’s ability to adequately problem solve and to understand the impact of each action on the learner, as well as sensitivity toward the learner’s experience.

We seek to prevent the abuses and potential repercussions of inappropriate, poorly applied, and inhumane uses of punishment and of overly-restrictive management and confinement strategies. The potential effects of punishment can include aggression or counter-aggression; suppressed behavior (preventing the trainer/consultant from adequately reading the animal); increased anxiety and fear; physical harm; a negative association with the owner or handler; increased unwanted behavior; and, new, unwanted behaviors. 5

LIMA guidelines require that trainer/consultants always offer the learner as much control and choice as possible. Trainer/consultants must treat each individual of any species with respect and awareness of the learner’s individual nature, preferences, abilities, and needs. 6

We focus on reinforcing desired behaviors, and always ask the question, “What do you want the animal to do?” Relying on punishment in training does not answer this question, and therefore offers no acceptable behavior for the animal to learn to replace the unwanted behavior. These LIMA guidelines do not justify the use of aversive methods and tools including, but not limited to, the use of electronic, choke or prong collars in lieu of other effective positive reinforcement interventions and strategies.

Intrusiveness refers to the degree to which a procedure affects the learners control. With a less intrusive procedure, a learner retains more control. The goal of LIMA is for its trainers/consultants to determine and use the least intrusive effective intervention which will effectively address the target behavior. In the course of an experienced trainer/consultant’s practice, he or she may identify a situation in which a relatively more intrusive procedure is necessary for an effective outcome. In such a case, a procedure that reduces the learner’s control may be the least intrusive, effective choice. Additionally, wellness is at the top of the hierarchy to ensure that a trainer/consultant does not implement a learning solution for behavior problems due to pain or illness. The hierarchy is a cautionary tool to reduce both dogmatic rule following and practice by familiarity or convenience. It offers an ethical checkpoint for consultants to carefully consider the process by which effective outcomes can be most humanely achieved on a case-by-case basis. The hierarchy is intended to be approached in order for each case. Rationale like, “It worked with the last case!” is not appropriate. The evaluation and behavior change program of every animal should be a study of the individual (i.e., individual animal, setting, caregiver, etc.). Changing behavior is best understood as a study of one.

1 Steven Lindsay, Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training Vol 3 pgs. 29 & 726. 2 Per the IAABC, APDT and CCPDT Joint Code of Conduct 3 “[The] use of positive reinforcement alone was associated with the lowest mean scores (attention- seeking score 0.33; fear (avoidance) score 0.18; aggression score 0.1). The highest mean attention-seeking score (0.49) was found in dogs whose owners used a combination of positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement. The highest mean avoidance score (0.31) was found in dogs whose owners used a combination of all categories of training method. Owners using a combination of positive reinforcement and positive punishment had dogs with the highest mean aggression score (0.27).” Emily J. Blackwell, Caroline Twells, Anne Seawright, Rachel A. Casey, The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs, Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, Volume 3, Issue 5, September–October 2008, Pages 207-217, ISSN 1558-7878, HTTP://DX.DOI.ORG/10.1016/J.JVEB.2007.10.008. 5 See avsabonline.org • Hutchinson RR. 1977. By-products of aversive control. In: Honig WK, Staddon JER, eds. Handbook of Operant Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall: 415-431.• Azrin NH. 1960. Effects of punishment intensity during variable-interval reinforcement. J Exp Analysis Behav 3: 123-142.• Azrin NH, Holz WC, Hake DR. 1963. Fixed-ratio punishment. J Exp Analysis Behav 6: 141-148. • Pauli AM, Bentley E, Diehl AK, Miller PE. 2006. Effects of the application of neck pressure by a collar or harness on intraocular pressure in dogs. J Am Anim Hosp Assoc 42(3): 207-211. • Drobatz KJ, Saunders HM, Pugh CR, Hendricks JC. 1995. Noncardiogenic pulmonary edema in dogs and cats: 26 cases (1987-1993). J Am Vet Med Assoc 206: 1732-1736. • Azrin NH, Rubin HB, Hutchinson RR. 1968. Biting attack by rats in response to aversive shock. J Exp Analysis Behav 11: 633-639. 6 Brambell’s Five Freedoms, used as animal and human welfare guidelines: • Freedom from hunger or thirst by ready access to fresh water and a diet to maintain full health and vigor • Freedom from discomfort by providing an appropriate environment including shelter and a comfortable resting area • Freedom from pain, injury or disease by prevention or rapid diagnosis and treatment • Freedom to express (most) normal behavior by providing sufficient space, proper facilities and company of the animal’s own kind • Freedom from fear and distress by ensuring conditions and treatment that avoids mental suffering 7 S. Friedman, What’s Wrong with this Picture? Effectiveness is Not Enough, APDT Journal March/April 2010 8 Position statement on humane dog training … – vet.osu.edu. (n.d.). Retrieved from https://vet.osu.edu/vmc/sites/default/files/files/companion/behavior/avsab-humane-dog-training-position-statement-2021.pdf 

Section 1

Least Instrusive, Minimally Aversive (LIMA)

“LIMA” is an acronym for the phrase “least intrusive, minimally aversive”. LIMA describes a trainer or behavior consultant who uses the least intrusive, minimally aversive strategy out of a set of humane and effective tactics likely to succeed in achieving a training or behavior change objective with minimal risk of producing aversive side effects. LIMA adherence also requires consultants to be adequately educated and skilled in order to ensure that the least intrusive and aversive procedure is used. 1 LIMA does not justify the use of punishment in lieu of other effective interventions and strategies. In the vast majority of cases, desired behavior change can be affected by focusing on the animal’s environment, physical well-being, and operant and classical interventions such as differential reinforcement of an alternative behavior, desensitization, and counter-conditioning.

LIMA requires trainers/consultants to work to increase the use of positive reinforcement and eliminate the use of punishment when working with animal and human clients. In order to ensure best practices, consultants should pursue and maintain competence in animal behavior consulting and training through continuing education, and hands-on experience. Trainers/consultants should not advise on problems outside the recognized boundaries of their competencies and experience. 2

Positive reinforcement should be the first line of teaching, training, and behavior change program considered, and should be applied consistently. Positive reinforcement is associated with the lowest incidence of aggression, attention seeking, avoidance, and fear in learners. 3 Only the learner determines what may be reinforcing. It is crucial that the trainer/consultant understands and has the ability to appropriately apply this principle. This fact may mean that the trainer/consultant assesses any handling, petting, food, tool, and environment each time the learner experiences them. Personal bias must not determine the learner’s experience. The measure of each stimulus is whether the learner’s target behavior is strengthening or weakening, not the trainer/consultant’s intent or preference.

The trainer/consultant is responsible for ensuring learner success through a consistent, systematic approach that identifies a specific target behavior, the purpose of that behavior, and the consequences that maintain the behavior. A variety of learning and behavior change strategies may come into play during a case. Ethical use of this variety always depends on the trainer/consultant’s ability to adequately problem solve and to understand the impact of each action on the learner, as well as sensitivity toward the learner’s experience.

We seek to prevent the abuses and potential repercussions of inappropriate, poorly applied, and inhumane uses of punishment and of overly-restrictive management and confinement strategies. The potential effects of punishment can include aggression or counter-aggression; suppressed behavior (preventing the trainer/consultant from adequately reading the animal); increased anxiety and fear; physical harm; a negative association with the owner or handler; increased unwanted behavior; and, new, unwanted behaviors. 5

LIMA guidelines require that trainer/consultants always offer the learner as much control and choice as possible. Trainer/consultants must treat each individual of any species with respect and awareness of the learner’s individual nature, preferences, abilities, and needs. 6

We focus on reinforcing desired behaviors, and always ask the question, “What do you want the animal to do?” Relying on punishment in training does not answer this question, and therefore offers no acceptable behavior for the animal to learn to replace the unwanted behavior. These LIMA guidelines do not justify the use of aversive methods and tools including, but not limited to, the use of electronic, choke or prong collars in lieu of other effective positive reinforcement interventions and strategies.

Intrusiveness refers to the degree to which a procedure affects the learners control. With a less intrusive procedure, a learner retains more control. The goal of LIMA is for its trainers/consultants to determine and use the least intrusive effective intervention which will effectively address the target behavior. In the course of an experienced trainer/consultant’s practice, he or she may identify a situation in which a relatively more intrusive procedure is necessary for an effective outcome. In such a case, a procedure that reduces the learner’s control may be the least intrusive, effective choice. Additionally, wellness is at the top of the hierarchy to ensure that a trainer/consultant does not implement a learning solution for behavior problems due to pain or illness. The hierarchy is a cautionary tool to reduce both dogmatic rule following and practice by familiarity or convenience. It offers an ethical checkpoint for consultants to carefully consider the process by which effective outcomes can be most humanely achieved on a case-by-case basis. The hierarchy is intended to be approached in order for each case. Rationale like, “It worked with the last case!” is not appropriate. The evaluation and behavior change program of every animal should be a study of the individual (i.e., individual animal, setting, caregiver, etc.). Changing behavior is best understood as a study of one.

1 Steven Lindsay, Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training Vol 3 pgs. 29 & 726. 2 Per the IAABC, APDT and CCPDT Joint Code of Conduct 3 “[The] use of positive reinforcement alone was associated with the lowest mean scores (attention- seeking score 0.33; fear (avoidance) score 0.18; aggression score 0.1). The highest mean attention-seeking score (0.49) was found in dogs whose owners used a combination of positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement. The highest mean avoidance score (0.31) was found in dogs whose owners used a combination of all categories of training method. Owners using a combination of positive reinforcement and positive punishment had dogs with the highest mean aggression score (0.27).” Emily J. Blackwell, Caroline Twells, Anne Seawright, Rachel A. Casey, The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs, Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, Volume 3, Issue 5, September–October 2008, Pages 207-217, ISSN 1558-7878, HTTP://DX.DOI.ORG/10.1016/J.JVEB.2007.10.008. 5 See avsabonline.org • Hutchinson RR. 1977. By-products of aversive control. In: Honig WK, Staddon JER, eds. Handbook of Operant Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall: 415-431.• Azrin NH. 1960. Effects of punishment intensity during variable-interval reinforcement. J Exp Analysis Behav 3: 123-142.• Azrin NH, Holz WC, Hake DR. 1963. Fixed-ratio punishment. J Exp Analysis Behav 6: 141-148. • Pauli AM, Bentley E, Diehl AK, Miller PE. 2006. Effects of the application of neck pressure by a collar or harness on intraocular pressure in dogs. J Am Anim Hosp Assoc 42(3): 207-211. • Drobatz KJ, Saunders HM, Pugh CR, Hendricks JC. 1995. Noncardiogenic pulmonary edema in dogs and cats: 26 cases (1987-1993). J Am Vet Med Assoc 206: 1732-1736. • Azrin NH, Rubin HB, Hutchinson RR. 1968. Biting attack by rats in response to aversive shock. J Exp Analysis Behav 11: 633-639. 6 Brambell’s Five Freedoms, used as animal and human welfare guidelines: • Freedom from hunger or thirst by ready access to fresh water and a diet to maintain full health and vigor • Freedom from discomfort by providing an appropriate environment including shelter and a comfortable resting area • Freedom from pain, injury or disease by prevention or rapid diagnosis and treatment • Freedom to express (most) normal behavior by providing sufficient space, proper facilities and company of the animal’s own kind • Freedom from fear and distress by ensuring conditions and treatment that avoids mental suffering 7 S. Friedman, What’s Wrong with this Picture? Effectiveness is Not Enough, APDT Journal March/April 2010 8 Position statement on humane dog training … – vet.osu.edu. (n.d.). Retrieved from https://vet.osu.edu/vmc/sites/default/files/files/companion/behavior/avsab-humane-dog-training-position-statement-2021.pdf 

Section 1

Least Instrusive, Minimally Aversive (LIMA)

“LIMA” is an acronym for the phrase “least intrusive, minimally aversive”. LIMA describes a trainer or behavior consultant who uses the least intrusive, minimally aversive strategy out of a set of humane and effective tactics likely to succeed in achieving a training or behavior change objective with minimal risk of producing aversive side effects. LIMA adherence also requires consultants to be adequately educated and skilled in order to ensure that the least intrusive and aversive procedure is used. 1 LIMA does not justify the use of punishment in lieu of other effective interventions and strategies. In the vast majority of cases, desired behavior change can be affected by focusing on the animal’s environment, physical well-being, and operant and classical interventions such as differential reinforcement of an alternative behavior, desensitization, and counter-conditioning.

LIMA requires trainers/consultants to work to increase the use of positive reinforcement and eliminate the use of punishment when working with animal and human clients. In order to ensure best practices, consultants should pursue and maintain competence in animal behavior consulting and training through continuing education, and hands-on experience. Trainers/consultants should not advise on problems outside the recognized boundaries of their competencies and experience. 2

Positive reinforcement should be the first line of teaching, training, and behavior change program considered, and should be applied consistently. Positive reinforcement is associated with the lowest incidence of aggression, attention seeking, avoidance, and fear in learners. 3 Only the learner determines what may be reinforcing. It is crucial that the trainer/consultant understands and has the ability to appropriately apply this principle. This fact may mean that the trainer/consultant assesses any handling, petting, food, tool, and environment each time the learner experiences them. Personal bias must not determine the learner’s experience. The measure of each stimulus is whether the learner’s target behavior is strengthening or weakening, not the trainer/consultant’s intent or preference.

The trainer/consultant is responsible for ensuring learner success through a consistent, systematic approach that identifies a specific target behavior, the purpose of that behavior, and the consequences that maintain the behavior. A variety of learning and behavior change strategies may come into play during a case. Ethical use of this variety always depends on the trainer/consultant’s ability to adequately problem solve and to understand the impact of each action on the learner, as well as sensitivity toward the learner’s experience.

We seek to prevent the abuses and potential repercussions of inappropriate, poorly applied, and inhumane uses of punishment and of overly-restrictive management and confinement strategies. The potential effects of punishment can include aggression or counter-aggression; suppressed behavior (preventing the trainer/consultant from adequately reading the animal); increased anxiety and fear; physical harm; a negative association with the owner or handler; increased unwanted behavior; and, new, unwanted behaviors. 5

LIMA guidelines require that trainer/consultants always offer the learner as much control and choice as possible. Trainer/consultants must treat each individual of any species with respect and awareness of the learner’s individual nature, preferences, abilities, and needs. 6

We focus on reinforcing desired behaviors, and always ask the question, “What do you want the animal to do?” Relying on punishment in training does not answer this question, and therefore offers no acceptable behavior for the animal to learn to replace the unwanted behavior. These LIMA guidelines do not justify the use of aversive methods and tools including, but not limited to, the use of electronic, choke or prong collars in lieu of other effective positive reinforcement interventions and strategies.

Intrusiveness refers to the degree to which a procedure affects the learners control. With a less intrusive procedure, a learner retains more control. The goal of LIMA is for its trainers/consultants to determine and use the least intrusive effective intervention which will effectively address the target behavior. In the course of an experienced trainer/consultant’s practice, he or she may identify a situation in which a relatively more intrusive procedure is necessary for an effective outcome. In such a case, a procedure that reduces the learner’s control may be the least intrusive, effective choice. Additionally, wellness is at the top of the hierarchy to ensure that a trainer/consultant does not implement a learning solution for behavior problems due to pain or illness. The hierarchy is a cautionary tool to reduce both dogmatic rule following and practice by familiarity or convenience. It offers an ethical checkpoint for consultants to carefully consider the process by which effective outcomes can be most humanely achieved on a case-by-case basis. The hierarchy is intended to be approached in order for each case. Rationale like, “It worked with the last case!” is not appropriate. The evaluation and behavior change program of every animal should be a study of the individual (i.e., individual animal, setting, caregiver, etc.). Changing behavior is best understood as a study of one.

1 Steven Lindsay, Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training Vol 3 pgs. 29 & 726. 2 Per the IAABC, APDT and CCPDT Joint Code of Conduct 3 “[The] use of positive reinforcement alone was associated with the lowest mean scores (attention- seeking score 0.33; fear (avoidance) score 0.18; aggression score 0.1). The highest mean attention-seeking score (0.49) was found in dogs whose owners used a combination of positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement. The highest mean avoidance score (0.31) was found in dogs whose owners used a combination of all categories of training method. Owners using a combination of positive reinforcement and positive punishment had dogs with the highest mean aggression score (0.27).” Emily J. Blackwell, Caroline Twells, Anne Seawright, Rachel A. Casey, The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs, Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, Volume 3, Issue 5, September–October 2008, Pages 207-217, ISSN 1558-7878, HTTP://DX.DOI.ORG/10.1016/J.JVEB.2007.10.008. 5 See avsabonline.org • Hutchinson RR. 1977. By-products of aversive control. In: Honig WK, Staddon JER, eds. Handbook of Operant Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall: 415-431.• Azrin NH. 1960. Effects of punishment intensity during variable-interval reinforcement. J Exp Analysis Behav 3: 123-142.• Azrin NH, Holz WC, Hake DR. 1963. Fixed-ratio punishment. J Exp Analysis Behav 6: 141-148. • Pauli AM, Bentley E, Diehl AK, Miller PE. 2006. Effects of the application of neck pressure by a collar or harness on intraocular pressure in dogs. J Am Anim Hosp Assoc 42(3): 207-211. • Drobatz KJ, Saunders HM, Pugh CR, Hendricks JC. 1995. Noncardiogenic pulmonary edema in dogs and cats: 26 cases (1987-1993). J Am Vet Med Assoc 206: 1732-1736. • Azrin NH, Rubin HB, Hutchinson RR. 1968. Biting attack by rats in response to aversive shock. J Exp Analysis Behav 11: 633-639. 6 Brambell’s Five Freedoms, used as animal and human welfare guidelines: • Freedom from hunger or thirst by ready access to fresh water and a diet to maintain full health and vigor • Freedom from discomfort by providing an appropriate environment including shelter and a comfortable resting area • Freedom from pain, injury or disease by prevention or rapid diagnosis and treatment • Freedom to express (most) normal behavior by providing sufficient space, proper facilities and company of the animal’s own kind • Freedom from fear and distress by ensuring conditions and treatment that avoids mental suffering 7 S. Friedman, What’s Wrong with this Picture? Effectiveness is Not Enough, APDT Journal March/April 2010 8 Position statement on humane dog training … – vet.osu.edu. (n.d.). Retrieved from https://vet.osu.edu/vmc/sites/default/files/files/companion/behavior/avsab-humane-dog-training-position-statement-2021.pdf 

Section 2

Humane Hierarchy

The Humane Hierarchy serves to guide professionals in their decision-making process during training and behavior modification. Additionally, it assists owners and animal care professionals in understanding the standard of care to be applied in determining training practices and methodologies and the order of implementation for applying those training practices and methodologies. Hierarchy of Procedures for Humane and Effective Practice

1.

Health, nutritional, and physical factors: Ensure that any indicators for possible medical, nutritional, or health factors are addressed by a licensed veterinarian. The consultant should also address potential factors in the physical environment.

2.

Antecedents: Redesign setting events, change motivations, and add or remove discriminative stimuli (cues) for the problem behavior.

3.

Positive Reinforcement: Employ approaches that contingently deliver a consequence to increase the probability that the desired behavior will occur.

4.

Differential Reinforcement of Alternative Behavior: Reinforce an acceptable replacement behavior and remove the maintaining reinforcer for the problem behavior.

5.

Negative Punishment, Negative Reinforcement, or Extinction (these are not listed in any order of preference):

a

Negative Punishment – Contingently withdraw a positive reinforcer to reduce the probability that the problem behavior will occur.

b

Negative Reinforcement – Contingently withdraw an aversive antecedent stimulus to increase the probability that the right behavior will occur.

c

Extinction – Permanently remove the maintaining reinforcer to suppress the behavior or reduce it to baseline levels.

6.

Positive Punishment: Contingently deliver an aversive consequence to reduce the probability that the problem behavior will occur.

Section 2

Humane Hierarchy

The Humane Hierarchy serves to guide professionals in their decision-making process during training and behavior modification. Additionally, it assists owners and animal care professionals in understanding the standard of care to be applied in determining training practices and methodologies and the order of implementation for applying those training practices and methodologies. Hierarchy of Procedures for Humane and Effective Practice

1.

Health, nutritional, and physical factors: Ensure that any indicators for possible medical, nutritional, or health factors are addressed by a licensed veterinarian. The consultant should also address potential factors in the physical environment.

2.

Antecedents: Redesign setting events, change motivations, and add or remove discriminative stimuli (cues) for the problem behavior.

3.

Positive Reinforcement: Employ approaches that contingently deliver a consequence to increase the probability that the desired behavior will occur.

4.

Differential Reinforcement of Alternative Behavior: Reinforce an acceptable replacement behavior and remove the maintaining reinforcer for the problem behavior.

5.

Negative Punishment, Negative Reinforcement, or Extinction (these are not listed in any order of preference):

a)

Negative Punishment – Contingently withdraw a positive reinforcer to reduce the probability that the problem behavior will occur.

b)

Negative Reinforcement – Contingently withdraw an aversive antecedent stimulus to increase the probability that the right behavior will occur.

c)

Extinction – Permanently remove the maintaining reinforcer to suppress the behavior or reduce it to baseline levels.

6.

Positive Punishment: Contingently deliver an aversive consequence to reduce the probability that the problem behavior will occur.

Section 2

Humane Hierarchy

The Humane Hierarchy serves to guide professionals in their decision-making process during training and behavior modification. Additionally, it assists owners and animal care professionals in understanding the standard of care to be applied in determining training practices and methodologies and the order of implementation for applying those training practices and methodologies. Hierarchy of Procedures for Humane and Effective Practice

1.

Health, nutritional, and physical factors: Ensure that any indicators for possible medical, nutritional, or health factors are addressed by a licensed veterinarian. The consultant should also address potential factors in the physical environment.

2.

Antecedents: Redesign setting events, change motivations, and add or remove discriminative stimuli (cues) for the problem behavior.

3.

Positive Reinforcement: Employ approaches that contingently deliver a consequence to increase the probability that the desired behavior will occur.

4.

Differential Reinforcement of Alternative Behavior: Reinforce an acceptable replacement behavior and remove the maintaining reinforcer for the problem behavior.

5.

Negative Punishment, Negative Reinforcement, or Extinction (these are not listed in any order of preference):

a)

Negative Punishment – Contingently withdraw a positive reinforcer to reduce the probability that the problem behavior will occur.

b)

Negative Reinforcement – Contingently withdraw an aversive antecedent stimulus to increase the probability that the right behavior will occur.

c)

Extinction – Permanently remove the maintaining reinforcer to suppress the behavior or reduce it to baseline levels.

6.

Positive Punishment: Contingently deliver an aversive consequence to reduce the probability that the problem behavior will occur.

Section 3

Least Inhibitive, Functionally Effective (LIFE)

Introduction

The Least Intrusive, Minimally Aversive Model (LIMA)

Identifying procedures as being “least intrusive” is a focal point for proper LIMA-based training methods. However, “intrusive” is a term that tells us little about the effect of any event on behavior. To say that something is an intrusion or intrudes on an individual does not describe the impact on how they overtly respond. Likewise, to say that some event is intrusive on behavior still tells us next to nothing on how it changes those responses. At best, “intrusive” is a term that requires additional operationalization. While a handful of behaviorists have used the term “intrusive” procedurally (for instance, see Carter and Wheeler, 2005), it is still problematic for describing any procedural effect.
The focus of the LIMA model is on practices to limit. For instance, while it may not be clear what effect “intrusive” has, the LIMA acronym tells us to have less of it. Likewise, LIMA tells us to be minimal in our training method selection approach. What, then, should we do instead? For over half a century, the applied behavioral sciences have argued in favor of focusing on what individuals should do, not simply what they should not do (Winett and Winkler, 1972Cooper et al., 2020). At least part of any force-free training philosophy should focus on criteria we ought to use. In other words, to be an effective and behaviorally friendly training model, it needs to inform what practices and behaviors to select for, not against.
One of the biggest problems for the LIMA approach is the justification it has enabled for regularly using aversive stimuli or coercive training methods. Some of this is historical, which becomes more evident as we go back to Lindsay’s original statements. For instance: According to the least intrusive and minimally aversive (LIMA) model, aversives are ranked in terms of their relative severity and intrusiveness, requiring that the trainer apply a less aversive technique before advancing to a more aversive one.” (p. 29). Thus, Lindsay was making an argument for the use of coercive training techniques, with his handbook providing pictures and descriptions of various aversive training tools. For example, in another description, Lindsay states: The proper use of the prong collar as a shaping and polishing tool requires significant instruction, but with respect to basic control uses novice trainers can rapidly master the prong collar.” (p. 31). What becomes clear in these statements is that LIMA was not intended to be an attempt to minimize the use of aversive stimuli, as many modern force-free trainers have conceptualized. Instead, Lindsay intended LIMA to be a framework to help trainers select their aversive stimuli and tools. As one of the latter statements in the handbook concludes: Aversive procedures are legitimate and valuable tools for controlling undesirable behavior…” (p. 725). While LIMA has been a useful, simple philosophy to help identify and describe a reward-based, force-free animal training approach, it also has the aforementioned drawbacks. Nonetheless, these criticisms should be viewed as ways to help the animal training field and force-free training philosophy move forward. All fields adjust their principles in order to advance, and progress is typically made by identification of what should be done next. Therefore, the section below details a potential new force-free training philosophy.

The Least Inhibitive, Functionally Effective Model (LIFE)

As discussed earlier, the use of the term “intrusive” tells us little about how any training method either affects the targeted animal or its behavior. Historically, behaviorists have relied on two terms to identify some type of reductive impact on behavior: (1) restriction, as specified in the least restrictive alternative (Johnston and Sherman, 1993) or the principle of least restrictiveness (Vollmer et al., 2011), and (2) inhibition, as detailed by the concept of conditioned inhibition (Rescorla, 1969) and inhibitory stimuli/effects (Domjan, 2020). Both terms, while abstractly tell us only that behavior was prevented in some way, can also inform us about a reduction in available and meaningful choices. Our goal, from a welfare perspective, should be to maximize available choices that are beneficial to any animal. Additionally, behaviorists have used a similar concept in “degrees of freedom” (Goldiamond, 1976de Fernandes and Dittrich, 2018) to identify genuine choices. The concept of degrees of freedom, as presented within Goldiamond’s (1974) constructional approach (see also Layng, 2009), can more generally be described as specifying the restriction or inhibition of behavioral repertoires, either through a lack of options (i.e., absence of alternative contingencies) or through force or coercion. We could equally provide a more inhibitive environment by reducing meaningful choices (e.g., environmental restriction through social isolation or food deprivation) or by applying aversive stimuli to force some response(s) (i.e., the use of choke, prong, or shock collars). Note that, in the first example, a trainer could say, “but I only use positive reinforcement!” even though their training practice may be inhibitive through environmental restriction. Choice, context, and function are all equally important parts of the LIFE model.
A cornerstone of many applied behavioral sciences, including animal welfare research and practice, has been a reliance on understanding and addressing the function of some behavior. In the field of applied behavior analysis, this has typically been done through the use of functional behavior assessments (Cooper et al., 2020), which have included functional analyses (Iwata et al., 1982Mace, 1994Hanley, 2012). The critical feature here is that understanding of the function of some behavior is connected to our proposed behavior change plans, so that newly trained behaviors can serve the same function as the originally offered responses. In other words, our ability to identify what causes some behavior means we are also more likely to successfully change it through appropriately matched contingencies. For example, if a trainer knows that a dog gets attention for barking when the doorbell rings, they can both stop giving attention for barking in the presence of a ringing doorbell, and just as important, give attention for an alternative response, such as sitting quietly on a mat (for instance, see Yin et al., 2008). Interventions such as rewarding an alternative response are reliant on matching our understanding of function to changing behavior: we know what causes the response, so we can choose to deliver similar contingencies for different actions. It also illustrates why emphasizing the importance of function in our model is imperative, since knowledge of function means we are more likely to change behavior successfully and ethically. For instance, it moves us beyond a “just ignore it” protocol, which, in the absence of rewarding other behaviors, may not help. Extinction alone does not provide functional alternatives and has been associated with problem behaviors, such as extinction bursts and aggression (Looney and Cohen, 1982Lerman and Iwata, 1995).

Defining success as more than being effective

Section 3

Least Inhibitive, Functionally Effective (LIFE)

Introduction

The Least Intrusive, Minimally Aversive Model (LIMA)

Identifying procedures as being “least intrusive” is a focal point for proper LIMA-based training methods. However, “intrusive” is a term that tells us little about the effect of any event on behavior. To say that something is an intrusion or intrudes on an individual does not describe the impact on how they overtly respond. Likewise, to say that some event is intrusive on behavior still tells us next to nothing on how it changes those responses. At best, “intrusive” is a term that requires additional operationalization. While a handful of behaviorists have used the term “intrusive” procedurally (for instance, see Carter and Wheeler, 2005), it is still problematic for describing any procedural effect.
The focus of the LIMA model is on practices to limit. For instance, while it may not be clear what effect “intrusive” has, the LIMA acronym tells us to have less of it. Likewise, LIMA tells us to be minimal in our training method selection approach. What, then, should we do instead? For over half a century, the applied behavioral sciences have argued in favor of focusing on what individuals should do, not simply what they should not do (Winett and Winkler, 1972Cooper et al., 2020). At least part of any force-free training philosophy should focus on criteria we ought to use. In other words, to be an effective and behaviorally friendly training model, it needs to inform what practices and behaviors to select for, not against.

One of the biggest problems for the LIMA approach is the justification it has enabled for regularly using aversive stimuli or coercive training methods. Some of this is historical, which becomes more evident as we go back to Lindsay’s original statements. For instance:

According to the least intrusive and minimally aversive (LIMA) model, aversives are ranked in terms of their relative severity and intrusiveness, requiring that the trainer apply a less aversive technique before advancing to a more aversive one.” (p. 29).

Thus, Lindsay was making an argument for the use of coercive training techniques, with his handbook providing pictures and descriptions of various aversive training tools. For example, in another description, Lindsay states:

The proper use of the prong collar as a shaping and polishing tool requires significant instruction, but with respect to basic control uses novice trainers can rapidly master the prong collar.” (p. 31).

What becomes clear in these statements is that LIMA was not intended to be an attempt to minimize the use of aversive stimuli, as many modern force-free trainers have conceptualized. Instead, Lindsay intended LIMA to be a framework to help trainers select their aversive stimuli and tools. As one of the latter statements in the handbook concludes:

Aversive procedures are legitimate and valuable tools for controlling undesirable behavior…” (p. 725).

While LIMA has been a useful, simple philosophy to help identify and describe a reward-based, force-free animal training approach, it also has the aforementioned drawbacks. Nonetheless, these criticisms should be viewed as ways to help the animal training field and force-free training philosophy move forward. All fields adjust their principles in order to advance, and progress is typically made by identification of what should be done next. Therefore, the section below details a potential new force-free training philosophy.

The Least Inhibitive, Functionally Effective Model (LIFE)

As discussed earlier, the use of the term “intrusive” tells us little about how any training method either affects the targeted animal or its behavior. Historically, behaviorists have relied on two terms to identify some type of reductive impact on behavior: (1) restriction, as specified in the least restrictive alternative (Johnston and Sherman, 1993) or the principle of least restrictiveness (Vollmer et al., 2011), and (2) inhibition, as detailed by the concept of conditioned inhibition (Rescorla, 1969) and inhibitory stimuli/effects (Domjan, 2020). Both terms, while abstractly tell us only that behavior was prevented in some way, can also inform us about a reduction in available and meaningful choices. Our goal, from a welfare perspective, should be to maximize available choices that are beneficial to any animal. Additionally, behaviorists have used a similar concept in “degrees of freedom” (Goldiamond, 1976de Fernandes and Dittrich, 2018) to identify genuine choices. The concept of degrees of freedom, as presented within Goldiamond’s (1974) constructional approach (see also Layng, 2009), can more generally be described as specifying the restriction or inhibition of behavioral repertoires, either through a lack of options (i.e., absence of alternative contingencies) or through force or coercion. We could equally provide a more inhibitive environment by reducing meaningful choices (e.g., environmental restriction through social isolation or food deprivation) or by applying aversive stimuli to force some response(s) (i.e., the use of choke, prong, or shock collars). Note that, in the first example, a trainer could say, “but I only use positive reinforcement!” even though their training practice may be inhibitive through environmental restriction. Choice, context, and function are all equally important parts of the LIFE model.
A cornerstone of many applied behavioral sciences, including animal welfare research and practice, has been a reliance on understanding and addressing the function of some behavior. In the field of applied behavior analysis, this has typically been done through the use of functional behavior assessments (Cooper et al., 2020), which have included functional analyses (Iwata et al., 1982Mace, 1994Hanley, 2012). The critical feature here is that understanding of the function of some behavior is connected to our proposed behavior change plans, so that newly trained behaviors can serve the same function as the originally offered responses. In other words, our ability to identify what causes some behavior means we are also more likely to successfully change it through appropriately matched contingencies. For example, if a trainer knows that a dog gets attention for barking when the doorbell rings, they can both stop giving attention for barking in the presence of a ringing doorbell, and just as important, give attention for an alternative response, such as sitting quietly on a mat (for instance, see Yin et al., 2008). Interventions such as rewarding an alternative response are reliant on matching our understanding of function to changing behavior: we know what causes the response, so we can choose to deliver similar contingencies for different actions. It also illustrates why emphasizing the importance of function in our model is imperative, since knowledge of function means we are more likely to change behavior successfully and ethically. For instance, it moves us beyond a “just ignore it” protocol, which, in the absence of rewarding other behaviors, may not help. Extinction alone does not provide functional alternatives and has been associated with problem behaviors, such as extinction bursts and aggression (Looney and Cohen, 1982Lerman and Iwata, 1995).

Defining success as more than being effective

Section 3

Least Inhibitive, Functionally Effective (LIFE)

Introduction

The Least Intrusive, Minimally Aversive Model (LIMA)

Identifying procedures as being “least intrusive” is a focal point for proper LIMA-based training methods. However, “intrusive” is a term that tells us little about the effect of any event on behavior. To say that something is an intrusion or intrudes on an individual does not describe the impact on how they overtly respond. Likewise, to say that some event is intrusive on behavior still tells us next to nothing on how it changes those responses. At best, “intrusive” is a term that requires additional operationalization. While a handful of behaviorists have used the term “intrusive” procedurally (for instance, see Carter and Wheeler, 2005), it is still problematic for describing any procedural effect.
The focus of the LIMA model is on practices to limit. For instance, while it may not be clear what effect “intrusive” has, the LIMA acronym tells us to have less of it. Likewise, LIMA tells us to be minimal in our training method selection approach. What, then, should we do instead? For over half a century, the applied behavioral sciences have argued in favor of focusing on what individuals should do, not simply what they should not do (Winett and Winkler, 1972Cooper et al., 2020). At least part of any force-free training philosophy should focus on criteria we ought to use. In other words, to be an effective and behaviorally friendly training model, it needs to inform what practices and behaviors to select for, not against.

One of the biggest problems for the LIMA approach is the justification it has enabled for regularly using aversive stimuli or coercive training methods. Some of this is historical, which becomes more evident as we go back to Lindsay’s original statements. For instance:

According to the least intrusive and minimally aversive (LIMA) model, aversives are ranked in terms of their relative severity and intrusiveness, requiring that the trainer apply a less aversive technique before advancing to a more aversive one.” (p. 29).

Thus, Lindsay was making an argument for the use of coercive training techniques, with his handbook providing pictures and descriptions of various aversive training tools. For example, in another description, Lindsay states:

The proper use of the prong collar as a shaping and polishing tool requires significant instruction, but with respect to basic control uses novice trainers can rapidly master the prong collar.” (p. 31).

What becomes clear in these statements is that LIMA was not intended to be an attempt to minimize the use of aversive stimuli, as many modern force-free trainers have conceptualized. Instead, Lindsay intended LIMA to be a framework to help trainers select their aversive stimuli and tools. As one of the latter statements in the handbook concludes:

Aversive procedures are legitimate and valuable tools for controlling undesirable behavior…” (p. 725).

While LIMA has been a useful, simple philosophy to help identify and describe a reward-based, force-free animal training approach, it also has the aforementioned drawbacks. Nonetheless, these criticisms should be viewed as ways to help the animal training field and force-free training philosophy move forward. All fields adjust their principles in order to advance, and progress is typically made by identification of what should be done next. Therefore, the section below details a potential new force-free training philosophy.

The Least Inhibitive, Functionally Effective Model (LIFE)

As discussed earlier, the use of the term “intrusive” tells us little about how any training method either affects the targeted animal or its behavior. Historically, behaviorists have relied on two terms to identify some type of reductive impact on behavior: (1) restriction, as specified in the least restrictive alternative (Johnston and Sherman, 1993) or the principle of least restrictiveness (Vollmer et al., 2011), and (2) inhibition, as detailed by the concept of conditioned inhibition (Rescorla, 1969) and inhibitory stimuli/effects (Domjan, 2020). Both terms, while abstractly tell us only that behavior was prevented in some way, can also inform us about a reduction in available and meaningful choices. Our goal, from a welfare perspective, should be to maximize available choices that are beneficial to any animal. Additionally, behaviorists have used a similar concept in “degrees of freedom” (Goldiamond, 1976de Fernandes and Dittrich, 2018) to identify genuine choices. The concept of degrees of freedom, as presented within Goldiamond’s (1974) constructional approach (see also Layng, 2009), can more generally be described as specifying the restriction or inhibition of behavioral repertoires, either through a lack of options (i.e., absence of alternative contingencies) or through force or coercion. We could equally provide a more inhibitive environment by reducing meaningful choices (e.g., environmental restriction through social isolation or food deprivation) or by applying aversive stimuli to force some response(s) (i.e., the use of choke, prong, or shock collars). Note that, in the first example, a trainer could say, “but I only use positive reinforcement!” even though their training practice may be inhibitive through environmental restriction. Choice, context, and function are all equally important parts of the LIFE model.
A cornerstone of many applied behavioral sciences, including animal welfare research and practice, has been a reliance on understanding and addressing the function of some behavior. In the field of applied behavior analysis, this has typically been done through the use of functional behavior assessments (Cooper et al., 2020), which have included functional analyses (Iwata et al., 1982Mace, 1994Hanley, 2012). The critical feature here is that understanding of the function of some behavior is connected to our proposed behavior change plans, so that newly trained behaviors can serve the same function as the originally offered responses. In other words, our ability to identify what causes some behavior means we are also more likely to successfully change it through appropriately matched contingencies. For example, if a trainer knows that a dog gets attention for barking when the doorbell rings, they can both stop giving attention for barking in the presence of a ringing doorbell, and just as important, give attention for an alternative response, such as sitting quietly on a mat (for instance, see Yin et al., 2008). Interventions such as rewarding an alternative response are reliant on matching our understanding of function to changing behavior: we know what causes the response, so we can choose to deliver similar contingencies for different actions. It also illustrates why emphasizing the importance of function in our model is imperative, since knowledge of function means we are more likely to change behavior successfully and ethically. For instance, it moves us beyond a “just ignore it” protocol, which, in the absence of rewarding other behaviors, may not help. Extinction alone does not provide functional alternatives and has been associated with problem behaviors, such as extinction bursts and aggression (Looney and Cohen, 1982Lerman and Iwata, 1995).

Defining success as more than being effective